TED Talks Worth Talking About | Beau Lotto’s Optical Illusions

http://www.ted.com/talks/lang/eng/beau_lotto_optical_illusions_show_how_we_see.html

beau lotto 120976_254x191

…nearly every living system has evolved the ability to detect light in one way or another. So, for us, seeing color is one of the simplest things the brain does. And yet, even at this most fundamental level, context is everything. What I want to talk about is not that context is everything, but why is context everything. Because it’s answering that question that tells us not only why we see what we do, but who we are as individuals, and who we are as a society.

But first, we have to ask another question, which is, “What is color for?”

So, color enables us to see the similarities and differences between surfaces, according to the full spectrum of light that they reflect. But what you’ve just done is, in many respects, mathematically impossible. Why? Because, as Barkley tells us, we have no direct access to our physical world, other than through our senses. And the light that falls on to our eyes is determined by multiple things in the world — not only the color of objects, but also the color of their illumination, and the color of the space between us and those objects. You vary any one of those parameters, and you’ll change the color of the light that falls onto your eye.

This is a huge problem because it means that the same image could have an infinite number of possible real-world sources. … Completely different meanings, giving rise to the exact same retinal information. And yet it’s only the retinal information that we get.

So how on earth do we even see? So, if you remember anything in this next 18 minutes, remember this: that the light that falls on to your eye, sensory information, is meaningless. Because it could mean literally anything. And what’s true for sensory information is true for information generally. There is no inherent meaning in information.It’s what we do with that information that matters.

So, how do we see? Well, we see by learning to see. So, the brain evolved the mechanisms for finding patterns, finding relationships in information, and associating those relationships with a behavioral meaning, a significance, by interacting with the world. We’re very aware of this in the form of more cognitive attributes, like language. So, I’m going to give you some letter strings. And I want you to read them out for me, if you can.

Ca    y u  rea   t  is?

Y ou  a e  not  r adi g  th s.

W at  ar   ou  rea in ?

Beau Lotto: “What are you reading?” Half the letters are missing. Right? There is no a priori reason why an “H” has to go between that “W” and “A.” But you put one there. Why? Because in the statistics of your past experience it would have been useful to do so. So you do so again. And yet you don’t put a letter after that first “T.” Why? Because it wouldn’t have been useful in the past. So you don’t do it again.

So let me show you how quickly our brains can redefine normality, even at the simplest thing the brain does, which is color. So, if I could have the lights down up here. I want you to first notice that those two desert scenes are physically the same. One is simply the flipping of the other. Okay? Now I want you to look at that dot between the green and the red. Okay? And I want you to stare at that dot. Don’t look anywhere else. And we’re going to look at that for about 30 seconds, which is a bit of a killer in and 18-minute talk.

Beau Lotto Desert Color Scene

But I really want you to learn. And I’ll tell you — don’t look anywhere else — and I’ll tell you what’s happening inside your head. Your brain is learning. And it’s learning that the right side of its visual field is under red illumination; the left side of its visual field is under green illumination. That’s what it’s learning. Okay? Now, when I tell you, I want you to look at the dot between the two desert scenes. So why don’t you do that now? (Laughter) Can I have the lights up again?

I take it from your response they don’t look the same anymore. Right? (Applause) Why? Because your brain is seeing that same information as if the right one is still under red light, and the left one is still under green light. That’s your new normal.

Beau Lotto Dark & Light Squares

So, what does this mean for context? It means that I can take these two identical squares, and I can put them in light and dark surrounds. And now the one on the dark surround looks lighter than the one on the light surround. What’s significant is not simply the light and dark surrounds that matter. It’s what those light and dark surrounds meant for your behavior in the past.

Beau Lotto Dark & Light Surrounds 1

So I’ll show you what I mean. Here we have that exact same illusion. We have two identical tiles, on the left, one in a dark surround, one in a light surround. And the same thing over on the right. Now, what I’m going to do is I’m going to review those two scenes. But I’m not going to change anything within those boxes, except their meaning. And see what happens to your perception.

Beau Lotto Dark & Light Surrounds 2

Notice that on the left the two tiles look nearly completely opposite: one very white and one very dark. Alright? Whereas, on the right, the two tiles look nearly the same. And yet there is still one on a dark surround, and one on a light surround. Why? Because if the tile in that shadow were in fact in shadow, and reflecting the same amount of light to your eye as the one outside the shadow, it would have to be more reflective — just the laws of physics. So you see it that way.

Whereas on the right, the information is consistent with those two tiles being under the same light. If they are under the same light, reflecting the same amount of light to your eye, then they must be equally reflective. So you see it that way. Which means we can bring all this information together to create some incredibly strong illusions.

Beau Lotto color cube 1

This is one I made a few years ago. And you’ll notice you see a dark brown tile at the top, and a bright orange tile at the side. That is your perceptual reality. The physical reality is that those two tiles are the same.

Beau Lotto color cube 2

Beau Lotto gray cubes 1

Here you see four gray tiles on your left, seven gray tiles on the right. I’m not going to change those tiles at all. But I’m going to reveal the rest of the scene. And see what happens to your perception. The four blue tiles on the left are gray. The seven yellow tiles on the right are also gray. They are the same. Okay? Don’t believe me? Let’s watch it again.

Beau Lotto gray cubes 2

What’s true for color is also true for complex perceptions of motion. So here we have — let’s turn this around — a diamond. And what I’m going to do is, I’m going to hold it here, and I’m going to spin it. And for all of you, you’ll see it probably spinning this direction. Now I want you to keep looking at it. Move your eyes around, blink, maybe close one eye. And suddenly it will flip, and start spinning the opposite direction. Yes? Raise your hand if you got that. Yes? Keep blinking. Every time you blink it will switch. Alright? So I can ask you, which direction is it rotating? How do you know? Your brain doesn’t know. Because both are equally likely. So depending on where it looks, it flips between the two possibilities.

Are we the only ones that see illusions? The answer to this question is no. Even the beautiful bumble bee, with its mere one million brain cells, which is 250 times fewer cells than you have in one retina, sees illusions, does the most complicated things that even our most sophisticated computers can’t do. So in my lab, we of course work on bumblebees. Because we can completely control their experience, and see how that alters the architecture of their brain. And we do this in what we call the Bee Matrix.

And here you have the hive. You can see the queen bee, that large bee in the middle there. Those are all her daughters, the eggs. And they go back and forth between this hive and the arena, via this tube. And you’ll see one of the bees come out here. You see how she has a little number on her? Yeah there is another one coming out. She has another number on her. Now, they are not born that way. Right? We pull them out, put them in the fridge, and they fall asleep. And then you can superglue little numbers on them. (Laughter)

And now in this experiment they get rewarded if they go to the blue flowers. And they land on the flower. They stick their tongue in there, called a proboscis, and they drink sugar water. Now she is drinking a glass of water that’s about that big to you and I, will do that about three times, and then fly. And sometimes they learn not to go to the blue, but to go to where the other bees go. So they copy each other. They can count to five. They can recognize faces. And here she comes down the ladder. And she’ll come into the hive, find an empty honey pot, and throw up, and that’s honey. (Laughter)

Now remember — (Laughter) — she’s supposed to be going to the blue flowers. But what are these bees doing in the upper right corner? It looks like they’re going to green flowers. Now, are they getting it wrong? And the answer to the question is no. Those are actually blue flowers. But those are blue flowers under green light. So they are using the relationships between the colors to solve the puzzle. Which is exactly what we do.

So, illusions are often used, especially in art, in the words of a more contemporary artist, “to demonstrate the fragility of our senses.” Okay, this is complete rubbish. The senses aren’t fragile. And if they were, we wouldn’t be here. Instead, color tells us something completely different, that the brain didn’t actually evolve to see the world the way it is. We can’t. Instead, the brain evolved to see the world the way it was useful to see in the past. And how we see is by continually redefining normality.

So how can we take this incredible capacity of plasticity of the brain and get people to experience their world differently? Well, one of the ways we do in my lab and studio is we translate the light into sound and we enable people to hear their visual world. And they can navigate the world using their ears.

Here is David, in the right. And he is holding a camera. On the left is what his camera sees. And you’ll see there is a line, a faint line going across that image. That line is broken up into 32 squares. In each square we calculate the average color. And then we just simply translate that into sound. And now he’s going to turn around, close his eyes, and find a plate on the ground, with his eyes closed.

He finds it. Amazing. Right? So not only can we create a prosthetic for the visually impaired, but we can also investigate how people literally make sense of the world. But we can also do something else. We can also make music with color. So, working with kids, they created images, thinking about what might the images you see sound like if we could listen to them. And then we translated these images. And this is one of those images. And this is a six-year-old child composing a piece of music for a 32 piece orchestra. And this is what it sounds like. So, a six year old child. Okay?

Now, what does all this mean? What this suggests is that no one is an outside observer of nature. Okay? We are not defined by our central properties, by the bits that make us up. We’re defined by our environment and our interaction with that environment — by our ecology. And that ecology is necessarily relative, historical and empirical. So what I’d like to finish with is this over here. Because what I’ve been trying to do is really celebrate uncertainty. Because I think only through uncertainty is there potential for understanding.

So, if some of you are still feeling a bit too certain, I’d like to do this one. So, if we have the lights down. And what we have here — Can everyone see 25 purple surfaces on your left, and 25, call it yellowish, surfaces on your right? So, now, what I want to do: I’m going to put the middle nine surfaces here under yellow illumination by simply putting a filter behind them. All right. Now you can see that changes the light that’s coming through there. Right? Because now the light is going through a yellowish filter and then a purplish filter. I’m going to do this opposite on the left here. I’m going to put the middle nine under purplish light.

Now, some of you will notice that the consequence is that the light coming through those middle nine on the right, or your left, is exactly the same as the light coming through the middle nine on your right. Agreed? Yes? Okay. So they are physically the same. Let’s pull the covers off. Now remember, you know the the middle nine are exactly the same. Do they look the same? No. The question is, “Is that an illusion?” And I’ll leave you with that. So, thank you very much. (Applause)

— VIA —

I’ve bolded the key pieces that I took away as most informative and intriguing, and I hope the screen shot images are useful for teaching this idea to others. The takeaways?

First, the ethic of humility, that we really don’t understand fully; that we “see in a glass/mirror dimly/darkly.” (1 Corinthians 13:12) And how we see everything ought to be tempered with a good dose of uncertainty, mystery, and open possibility. And, perhaps our epistemological ethic is rid ourselves of the pursuit of knowledge, and strive even more so for a relationship with the surrounding world. This sounds a lot like the ethic of compassion, the ethic and epistemology of love.

Second, most change is really adaptation. How can we begin to strive for true revolutionary change if the very senses that we use to see, to hear, to understand are mere adaptations and readjustments of normality? The idea of “vision” or “envisioning” the future seems much more challenging in light of this contextual understanding.

Last, and perhaps most important, I am more and more struck by the reality that CONTEXT IS EVERYTHING. In all disciplines, in all ethics, in all wisdom, and in all understanding, context rules. And to remove or extract pieces out of context is to dismiss truth, reality, and our personal and fundamental engagement with what is “real” in the world.

About Via

www.kevinneuner.com

3 comments

  1. TC

    If you inspect the images and isolate the squares he talks about on these, you can see his claims are false.

    Only the ones that look blue are actually gray when you isolate, and even then it looks like he’s added a tiny bit of blue as a suggestion. This is with the surroundings stripped out.

    Optical illusions exist, but that brown on the top is objectively darker than the light orange, no matter what, on a computer screen printout, even when you remove the surroundings.

    • Derek

      Totally wrong – anyone, with the most simple of tools can see they are objectively the same. Here is all you have to do. Save the pic above, open it in any picture manipulation tool (including paint), crop it so that only some of the top ‘brown’ square remains. Now open the full picture again in another window, and crop to only the ‘orange’ square on the side. Now put both of these, cropped images, laying partially on top of each other and they blend perfectly. You can do the same by printing and cutting out each square & comparing, but they are pretty small. Doing it electronically allows you to zoom in or enlarge to see for sure.

  2. You can also use the custom colors eyedropper tool to decode the red, green, blue component colors and see that what he is saying is true, your brain and your eyes can be tricked, that’s how magicians have made a living for centuries.

Leave a comment